Monday, October 14 -- Mating Systems

[Topic List] | [Next Lecture] | [Previous Lecture] | [Discussion]

Next time we will have a guest speaker on female mate choice.
Remember it was male-male competition or female mate choice that determined sexual selection, and we did male-male competition last time so you might think it would be logical to do female mate choice this time, but we're skipping ahead to mating systems since the guest can't make it 'til Wednesday.

About the test: It is written, not scantron or multiple choice. Mostly short answers, with a few essays. The emphasis will be on lectures. We're responsible for the readings, but they will not be emphasized.

Diversity of primate mating systems

Remember the terminology:
1m:1f=monogamy.
1m:Xf=polygyny.
1f:Xm=polyandry.
Xm:Xf=promiscuity

Prototypes from each of our primate groups:

Monogamy:
Prosimian- at least one species of tarsier.
New world monkey- titi monkey, night monkey, callitrichids.
Old world monkey- langur/mentawi island leaf monkey.
Ape- Gibbon.

Polygyny:
Prosimian- lemur.
New world monkey- howlers, cebus monkey.
Old world monkey- most of leaf monkeys, langurs, family presbytis, macaques.
Ape- gorilla.

Polyandry: (rare in primates)
New world monkey only- some of the callitrichids are "facultatively" polyandrous. This means they're not always so, but they can and in some cases tend to be.

Promiscuity:
New world monkey- marikis.
Apes- chimps.

What factors shape the evolution of mating systems?

Sexual selection:
Think about parental investment. Females do more than males especially in primates since they do gestation and lactation. So females become a limiting resource for males. Males can't increase their reproductive success by caring for their kids since they don't know who their kids are, but they can seek more matings.

In polygyny, males put forth more mating effort while females put forth more parental effort. In monogamy, both do more parental effort. In polygyny there's stronger or more intense sexual selection, but in monogamy it's not so intense. Usually in primates you don't see equal parental efforts because females have the ability and tendency to put more into parental effort since they gestate and lactate.

So, females go where the food is and males go where the females are. This is a good generalization for cases when male involvement is minor.

Ecological factors:
Environmental Potential for Polygyny (EPP)- how resources are distributed.

Females choose their limiting resource which is food and males choose theirs which is females. So males will always be under selection to monopolize multiple females. For instance whether they'll be able to get access to females depends on how females are dispersed in space and in time. If they're evenly distributed in space, it'll be hard to hold on to more than one female so there's little potential for polygamy. But if resources, and therefore females, are clumped then one male can control access to many females. In this case there's a lot of potential for males to remain polygynous.

Also females' distribution in time affects the EPP- if they are synchronized in breeding /estrous, then it's more difficult for a single male to monopolize females since they're all receptive at once and it's too easy for other guys to sneak in while he's working on one. If they're spread out in time then it's easier for the same male to control the few females who are ready at each time. Note that these two kinds of clumping have the opposite effect of each other. Synchrony reduces defendability of mates while spatial clumping increases defendability.

Polygyny

Two preconditions for the evolution of polygyny
1. It must be economically feasible for males to defend females. (Costs vs. Benefits.)
2. Males must be able to capitalize on potential for polygyny. This depends on characteristics that have evolved in their phylogeny, for instance, how much male parental care is required to raise offspring.

So, like, there might be high polygyny potential, but males might not be free to take advantage of it because babies will die without their contribution. Ecological factors determine both EPP and ability to capitalize on the EPP. Phylogenic factors also determine how well the male can capitalize on the EPP. Economic feasibility and capitalization ability both determine the degree of monopolization of mates which determines mating system.

(There's a flow chart diagram for this which makes it easier to understand than mere words)

Why most primates are polygynous

In most primate species, given the distribution of resources, females do usually congregate. You know, primates are mostly social. Because they're clustered but not in huge groups, they're both possible and beneficial to defend. Also important, females have long intervals means that they're receptive more or less one at a time so they're easier to defend.

To sum up:
"In most primate species, females congregate spatially in small, stable groups.
In addition, long interbirth intervals create a situation in which there are only a few reproductively active females per sexually active male.
These factors set the stage for intense male-male competition for the limited number of fertilizable females, and polygyny typically results."

Monogamy

Ok, so if polygyny is so great then why do some end up being monogamous? It's certainly not very common:
Birds- 90% monogamous.
Mammals- under 5% monogamous
Primates- 37/200=~18% monogamous.
(Traditional human societies are about 20% monogamous.)

Characteristics of monogamous primates

  1. Limited mating opportunities
  2. Male investment in offspring is high
  3. Male confidence in paternity is high (we're just talking probability here, not mental awareness)
  4. Little sexual dimorphism
  5. Territoriality & sex-specific aggression

Hypothesis for the evolution of monogamy

Evolves when males can only economically defend one female. This could be because of ecological constraints or because of the demands of parental care. Quite different from birds where parental care is the main factor. Someone always has to be brooding the eggs so they have to take turns so they can eat. This doesn't apply to primates so monogamy is driven more by the spatial distribution of females. Usually it's just too hard for males to defend more than one female.

To sum up:
"Monogamy evolves when either sex has the ability to monopolize multiple members of the opposite sex either because of ecological factors do not permit them to or because of the constraints imposed by parental care.
1. Monogamy evolves when male parental care is indispensable to female reproduction.
2. Monogamy evolves when aggression by mated females leads to their spatial separation and prevents males from acquiring additional mates."

#1 seems to be the case in new world monkeys. Crucial point is body size. They're really small but they have offspring that are large relative to the adult size to its really hard for a lone female to raise offspring. Males could abandon and mate more often, but they wouldn't end up passing along more genes.

In gibbons however, females are very evenly distributed in the environment and this seems to be because of mutual aggression between females. So a male might like to have a harem but the females won't have any of it. This has been difficult to prove experimentally, but they have done playback experiments. When you play female sounds then the female of a pair will charge over to the speaker to attack but the male will just sit there and be a dork. So the females reduce males' options until they have no choice but to be monogamous.


Promiscuity

Generally happens when males don't succeed in monopolizing access to any females. This may be because of the minimal mammalian social system where males are ranging widely through the home ranges of several females. Because they don't have defended territories, they can't monopolize any females. This is seen in the orangutans as well as some prosimians.

It also happens in large groups with multiple males and females. There may be two factors happening- it may not be economically feasible to restrict access to a large group, too big a job for one male. It has also been suggested that in like chimps and maybe marikis(brachyteles) it's that males need to reduce aggressive interactions between themselves because they've got to cooperate to defend joint territory from outsiders. They suppress their natural competition otherwise they'd all lose all the females. At least when they share they get some females. So this is all why males might be into promiscuity. What do the females get out of it?

Females must have a reason for mating with more than one male, too. Females' reasons haven't been studied as much. One reason might be to ensure fertilization. Also it might confuse paternity so it's not sure which male fathered her offspring so they're less likely to kill the kid. Or, maybe it's easier for a female to just mate with a male than to be subjected to aggression if she refuses. More on this later, but maybe it's just less risky for females to lie back and have done with it.

Polyandry

Rare in mammals in general, but occurs facultatively in tamarins.
Data from six populations of saddleback tamarins studied over 4 years.
Percentage of groups displaying each type of composition:
(X=multiple)
1f-1m 22%
1f-Xm 61%
Xf-Xm 14%
single-sex 3%

So maybe they're not completely polyandrous but it's the most common result. This may occur because they have unusual reproductive biology. For one thing they usually have twins. Offspring are quite large at birth; their combined weight can be up to 25% of the mother's weight. (OUCH!) By the time they wean, each offspring is about half the size of the mother. So she's providing enough milk to provide enough for two who together weigh as much as her! So females don't usually even carry offspring after second week. Males do this, or previous offspring. So males do much more carrying and seem to be completely necessary especially to escape from predators. Male parental care is limiting to reproductive success, and it's even higher with more than one male helping, so sometimes it takes two males just to raise one female's offspring.




Wednesday, October 16 -- Female Mate Choice

[Topic List] | [Next Lecture] | [Previous Lecture] | [Discussion]

Announcement: All overheads used in lecture are available on reserve now.
Exam update: It's all short answer, like a few sentences.

Our guest lecturer today is Rebecca Dowan. She's a doctoral student here at U of Michigan doing her thesis on female mate choice. So she's been studying it and reading all about it and everything.

Review of Sexual Selection Theory

Remember Darwin's two components- competition within one sex, like male-male competition, and choice of one sex for the other sex, as in female mate choice. Females invest more in offspring so they have more to lose by making a bad mate choice so you expect them to be more choosy. Females become limiting resource for males. We're going to be talking about females mate choice in primate species who live in social groups with more than one male in it, but we must remember that it also undoubtedly occurs in other types of social groups. In gorillas, for example, a female choosing which male's group to join also makes a mate choice.

Mate Choice

Note that this is not necessarily a conscious decision; it's just behavior. Imagine some species of fish. Females who are about to spawn swim in shallow water, and will mate with anyone who is there. Males compete to establish territory in the shallow water. So, the bigger guys get the women cause they can edge out the other, smaller fish. Females aren't necessarily consciously choosing the big guys, it's just the way they behave.

Mate choice may be defined as follows:

"Any pattern of behavior, shown by members of one sex, that leads to their being more likely to mate with certain members of the opposite sex than others."

Mate Choice Mechanisms

Active solicitation
Seen in the brown capuchin- females in estrous will follow males around, making distinct vocalizations and facial expressions.

Refusals to mate
In vervet monkeys, females will refuse to mate and will show aggression against courting males. In a study, over half of the courting males were refused, with 10-20% involving bites and chases by the females. Since vervet males aren't much larger, females can refuse pretty easily. Also, females will cooperate to defend other females from males. In rhesus macaques, females have also been found to successfully refuse.

Subtle signals
In savannah baboons, there is too much sexual dimorphism for females to refuse the males and exhibit aggression toward them. Males will herd estrous females in consortships, which are short-term pairbonds that can last 1-4 days, while the female is in peak estrous. Consortships are characterized by the male's maintaining close proximity to the female. The female can decide where she wants to go, but the male follows. They are also characterized by a high frequency of mating, usually initiated by males. Also, there is a high frequency of males grooming females. (Usually, females groom males.) Although males in consort herd partners from other males, sometime a female will persistently approach other males, often of a lower rank than her current partner. Our lecturer has been studying whether or not females' approaches affect the likelihood of the consortship being challenged.

What traits might a female select for in a mate?

Dominance rank
Traditional sexual selection theory predicts that females will mate with dominant males- they've shown themselves to be superior, so they must have superior genes. For example, in brown capuchins, the females solicit exclusively the dominant male, only switching to subordinates after peak fertility. This has little to do with the males' genetic qualities and more to do with their willingness and ability to protect children they have fathered. Proof: Recently deposed adult males are suddenly no longer attractive to females. Their genes haven't changed, only their rank.

In rhesus macaques, females will maintain proximity to the lower ranking males. This has also been observed in Japanese macaques and some baboon populations. Also, in macaques in captivity, blood testes were used to establish paternity and rank was shown to not correlate with number of children fathered. Conclusion: There's a lot of variation.

Unfamiliarity
In langurs, patas monkeys, redtails, and blue monkeys (all polygynous species), females have been observed to mate selectively with newly transferred males. Why? Maybe to avoid harmful effects of inbreeding. In groups where known males are likely to be related to the females, it's better to mate with foreign guys.

Another reason could be to prevent infanticide, which is typically committed by males who have just entered breeding system. Pregnant females may solicit copulation with the new guy to make him think that maybe her kid is his. Among the baboons at Gombe, females solicit more from males trying to enter than from established males. In fact, sometimes females will go a really long way to solicit new males.
Our lecturer has also seen this in the captive population that she's been watching. Established males will herd females away from new males, but the females will be persistent in following and bugging the new guys.

In Japanese macaques, female mate choice is responsible for circulation between groups. As time goes on, males rise in group rank but they also become less attractive to the females. So they leave for a new group after a few years because even though they will be of a lower rank, they'll get more sex.


"Good genes"
This is the most intuitively solid theory, but there's little evidence for it. In theory, females could prefer males whose genes would give their offspring good traits like health or strength. Or, they could be choosing males with some other type of (ornamental) characteristic which makes them attractive. Male squirrel monkeys, for example, seasonally increase their body size by like 25% using water and fat. Females will actively choose the really big males so maybe this seasonal weight gain is a case of "good" ornamental genes.


Variety of mates
Having a variety of mates is good for two reasons. First, it may prevent infanticide because since she did it with so many guys they don't know for sure that the kid isn't theirs. Second, it could induce multiple males to take care of the kid.

This may be what is happening in Barbary macaques. Estrous females mate with numerous males every day, and the males do not interfere with each others' copulations. Males are also involved with a lot of infant care. There's not much evidence that males do more protection of infants that they're more likely to have fathered.

Return benefits
From Smuts' work with savannah baboons; certain male-female baboon pairs spend a lot of time grooming and spending time together at times when she's not sexually active, like when she's pregnant or lactating. these pairs are called friends. Females get defense from the males- males protect both her and her infants even when there's no way he could be the father. Males also hold, carry, and groom kids of their friends, as well as allow them access to good feeding areas. The males benefit by using their friends' kids as buffers against attacks from other males. Also, when the female does eventually come back into estrous, she'll be more likely to mate with her friend than with some other guy. So, she may chose who to mate with based on benefits she got or expects to get from this behavior.

The difference between choice and preference

This is an important side note. Sexual selection is a hot topic in animal behavior these days, but it's only recently that we've moved from studying male-male competition to female mate choice. This is due in part to empirical problems with figuring out how to separate the effects of female mate choice and male behavior.

You will remember that the different sexes have often conflicting interests. An observed behavior might not represent male and female 's interests evenly. Often female mate choice will be constrained by male's tactics.

Preference is desires or propensities than an individual possesses. Preference does not always result in choice. Compare this to the definition above of choice.

So female mate choice is an action that can be observed and measured- it's what the female actually does. However, preference can't be so easily measured. So far investigators have only been able to measure observable behavior. We don't know how much the choice reflects females' actual preferences, since observed mating is usually a compromise between female and male interests. So female preferences can only be studied experimentally.

So how might male behavior constrain female choice?

Male-male competition; In baboons, it's especially intense. Dominant males have a lot of influence over what lower males do. Lower males might not be allowed to get near females so an observer can't tell whether the females prefer anyone but the highest-ranking males.
Male coercion of females; It is very common for males attack estrous females. In one group being studied, it occurs an average of 5 times a week. One in fifty attacks results in serious injury. Males will often use aggression to initiate and maintain consortships, as well. So, females might be constrained by this too.
But none of this has been tested. This is what our lecture set out to figure out;

To what extent do male tactics constrain female preferences?

The study group was a captive population of baboons in University of Washington's primate field station. The baboons live in a four-acre enclosure in a naturalistic setting.

She basically collected daily observations, documenting who the females were mating with, who they formed consortships with. The focus was on consort pairs. Outwardly, consort pairs suggest that they're dictated by males more than by females. So she compared this data with that from experiments to test whether females have mating preferences at all, and if so, what they are. Does the female baboon like the guy she's in consortship with?

To do the experiments, she set up a room with three males isolated in cage areas that permit some movement. Dividers prevented the males from touching or seeing each other, but they know of each others' presence through smell and vocalizations, of course. A female is put into the room for a two hour period. Then the observers note who she approaches, who she presents to, who she grooms and hangs out with. In this way, she has greatly reduced the effects of male-male competition and male coercion. Of course, the males can and do still threaten the females, but they can't physically hurt the females.

No statistical results yet because the data collection has just finished, but the females seem to be doing different things than in the group. They do indeed seem to have a preference, often for someone other than their normal consort. Also some females demonstrate a sampler type of preference, checking out all their options pretty equally. Maybe they' like to mate with multiple males. This seems to imply that the females are constrained by male behaviors.

Why do this? Until we know to what extent male tactics constrain female preference, we don't correctly understand female mate choice.




Friday, October 18 -- The Midterm

[Topic List] | [Next Lecture] | [Previous Lecture] | [Discussion]




Discussion --


[Topic List] | [Next Lecture]

Today we handed in our papers and reviewed some more for the midterm.



Let me know your thoughts: phyl@umich.edu
Last modified: October, 1996